Research Article | | Peer-Reviewed

Serial Verbs Constructions as Double-headed Syntactic Structures: Evidence from Igbo

Received: 20 October 2024     Accepted: 4 November 2024     Published: 22 November 2024
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

Although serial verb constructions (SVCs) are widely observed across languages, there is ongoing debate regarding their syntactic representation, particularly in cases involving shared arguments. Two prominent approaches to SVCs with argument sharing are the double-headed structure proposed by Baker (1989) and the control structure suggested by Collins (1997). This paper presents novel data from Igbo to evaluate these differing approaches. First, we disentangle the confound between covert coordination and SVCs through syntactic and semantic tests, demonstrating that SVCs do exist in Igbo. We then conduct a comprehensive comparison between SVCs and corresponding verb-verb (V-V) compounds. The absence of a systematic derivational relationship between SVCs and V-V compounds raises doubts about the verb incorporation mechanism posited by Collins. Furthermore, the lack of resultative SVCs in Igbo challenges the existence of an empty category, further undermining the control structure for SVCs. In contrast, we illustrate that the endocentric property of Igbo SVCs aligns naturally with Baker's double-headed syntactic structure. Building on this, we propose a double-headed structure for SVCs in Igbo, both with and without shared arguments. Our findings thus provide a novel argument for a permissible structure of SVCs with argument sharing and for the availability of double-headedness in Universal Grammar.

Published in International Journal of Language and Linguistics (Volume 12, Issue 5)
DOI 10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13
Page(s) 187-193
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Serial Verb Constructions, Igbo, Double-Headedness, Argument Sharing

1. Introduction
Serial verb constructions (SVCs) are widely observed in West African languages. Following Aikhenvald (2006), this paper defines SVCs as follows.
“A serial verb construction is a sequence of verbs that acts together as a single predicate, without any overt marker of conjunction, subordination, or syntactic dependency of any sort. They are mono-clausal; their intonation properties are the same as those of a mono-verbal clause, and they just have one tense, aspect, and polarity value. Serial verb construction may also share core and other arguments. Each component of an SVC must be able to occur on its own right.”
Among various types of SVCs, SVCs involving argument sharing have been a core issue in generative syntax. A typical example of SVCs with argument sharing is illustrated by a resultative in Ewe. As in (1), the two verbs “nya” and “dzo” share an argument sandwiched between them.
(1) Me nya ɖevi-ɛ dzo
1SG chase child-DEF leave
'I chased the child away.'
not 'I chased the child and I left.'
There are various approaches to argument sharing in SVCs. Baker (1989) proposes a ternary-branching structure as in Figure 1 [8], where a double-headed verb phrase allows an argument shared by two verbs. Hiraiwa & Bodomo (2008) offer a variant of this approach in which the verbs of an SVC are in separate constituents, but the shared argument is the constituent of more than one verb phrase . Collins (1997) analyzes SVCs as control structures . Based on data from Ewe, he maintains that argument sharing is mediated by the presence of a null pronoun pro, which must be controlled by the object of the first verb, shown in Figure 2. To rule out the impossible reading of (1) generated by the structure in Figure 3, where the pro is controlled by the matrix subject, Collins claims that the second verb must incorporate into the first verb in an SVC at Logical Form. Thus the structure in Figure 3 is ruled out since incorporation is blocked as V1 does not c-command V2.
Figure 1. Double-headed structure by Baker (1989).
Figure 2. Control structure by Collins (1997).
Figure 3. The structure of the infelicitous reading of (1).
Against this backdrop, we investigate SVCs in Igbo. Igbo has been claimed to have both verb-verb (V-V) compounds and SVCs . For instance, the compound verb “g͡ba-bà” in (2) can be decomposed into two independent verbs shown in (3). Therefore, Igbo provides a window to examine verb incorporation as well as different analyses of argument sharing.
(2) Obi g͡ba-bà-rà ọhịa
Obi run-enter-rV bush
'Obi run into the bush.'
(3) Òbi g͡bà-rà ọsọ bà-a n' ọhịa
Obi run-rV running enter-OVS P bush
'Obi ran and entered the bush.'
This paper investigates the relationship between SVCs and corresponding V-V compounds in Igbo and finds that in all types of SVCs with V-V counterparts, there are no systematic patterns indicating a derivational relationship. Moreover, the lack of resultative SVCs casts doubt on the existence of a pro. On the other hand, SVCs in Igbo are semantically endocentric as V-V compounds, which can be naturally analyzed by double-headedness. Therefore, our data provide support for Baker's insight of the double-headedness and challenge Collin's control structure.
The rest of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 disentangles the confound between SVCs and covert coordination in Igbo and shows that SVCs exist in Igbo. Section 3 examines the derivational relationship between the V-V compound and SVCs for each type of SVCs. Section 4 discusses the imcompatibility of a control structure and proposes a double-headed structure for SVCs based on their endocentric property.
2. SVCs and (c) Overt Coordination
There has been a debate about whether Igbo has SVCs or not. Lord (1977) did a typological study on the Niger-Congo language family and claims that Igbo appears to be unique with respect to predicate type by having widespread V-V compounds without SVCs . This view has been challenged by recent studies. For example, Obiamalu & Mbagwu (2014) provide (3) to show that V-V compounds can be decomposed into SVCs . However, they also have the following examples showing that V-V compounds can be decomposed into two sentences with a pause in between, e.g., (5), or coordinated structures indicated by an optional conjunction marker “ma”, e.g., (7). Therefore, it is not clear whether (3) is a real SVCs or covert coordination. Previous studies on SVCs in Igbo did not distinguish those two. This section provides tests to demonstrate that SVCs differ from coordination syntactically and semantically such that SVCs exist in Igbo.
(4) Àda tụ-fù-rù akwụkwọ
Ada throw-lose-rV book
'Ada lost a book.'
(5) Àda tụ̀-rù akwụkwo, ò fu-o
Ada throw-lose-rV book 3SG got.lost-OVS
'Ada threw away a book and it got lost.'
(6) Òbi kụ̀-gbù-rù agwọ
Obi hit-kill-rV snake
'Obi hit to death a snake.'
(7) Òbi kụ̀-rụ̀ agwọ ihē (ma) gbu-o yā
Obi hit-rV snake thing and kill-OVS 3SG
'Obi hit a snake with something and killed it.'
2.1. Syntactic Evidence for SVCs
2.1.1. Object Sharing
In Igbo, two verbal phrases can be coordinated by the conjunction marker “ma”. When two coordinated verbs have coindexed objects as in (9), the object of the second verb cannot be deleted under identity, shown by the obligatory pronoun “ja”, which indicates that structures like (8) with object sharing are real SVCs.
(8) Àdá zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀ sí-é
Ada buy-rV chicken cook-OVS
'Ada bought a chicken and cooked it.'
(9) Àdá zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀i mà sí-é *(jāi)
Ada buy-rV chicken and cook-OVS 3SG
'Ada bought a chicken and cooked it.'
2.1.2. The Scope of Negation
As shown in (10), in SVCs, the negation maker “-ghi” is attached to the first verb but it scopes over the entire clause.
(10) t͡ʃiɔ́má é-dʒì-g͡hì ḿ̩mà g͡bú-ó ɔ̄kʊ́kɔ̀
Chioma ?-use-NEG knife kill-OVS chicken
'Chioma didn’t kill the chicken with a knife.
In coordination like (11), however, each conjunct needs to be negated separately. The contrast between (10) and (11) indicates that SVCs are distinct from (c)overt coordination.
(11) Àdá á-zʊ̄-ghɪ̄ ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀, ò sí-ghɪ̄ jā, mà
Ada ?-buy-NEG chicken 3SG cook-NEG 3SG and
ò ɹí-ghī jā
3SG eat-NEG 3SG
'Ada didn't buy, cook or eat a chicken.'
2.1.3. Tense and Aspect Marker
In coordination such as (12) and (14), the future tense marker “ga” and progressive marker “na” occurring in the first conjunct need to be repeated in the second conjunct. However, they can be shared in SVCs without repetition, as in (13) and (15).
(12) ɔ́ nà à-gbá ɔ́sɔ̄ mà (ná) é-gè égʷū
3SG PROG ?-run race and PROG ?-listen music
'He is running and listening to music.'
(13) ɔ́ nà à-gbá ɔ́sɔ̄ é-gè égʷū
3SG PROG ?-run race ?-listen music
'He is running listening to music.'
(14) Ada ga-a-zʊ ɔkʊkɔ, *(ga-e) si ja, ma
Ada FUT-?-buy chicken, FUT-? cook 3SG, and
*(ga-e) ɹi ja
(FUT-?) eat 3SG
'Ada will buy a chicken, cook, and eat it.'
(15) t͡ʃiɔ́má gà-é-dʒì ḿ̩mà g͡bú-ó ɔ̄kʊ̄kɔ̀
Chioma FUT-?-use knife kill-OVS chicken
'Chioma will use a knife to kill a chicken.'
2.2. Semantic Evidence for SVCs
Semantically, SVCs express a single event . In directional SVCs, as in (16), the first verb expresses a manner of motion while the second verb expresses a direction. The two verbs can only be perceived as subparts of a single event and cannot be coordinated.
(16) ɔ́ gà-ɹà ídʒè (*ma) báɲé ná-ímé ʊ́lɔ̀
3SG walk-rV walk and enter inside house
'He walked and entered the house.'
On the other hand, For SVCs that have coordinated counterparts, the coordinated sentence is interpreted differently as involving separate events. For instance, when the benefactive SVC in (17) is expressed by coordination in (18), the indirect object of “ɲe” is no longer interpreted as the benefactive of “zʊ”, shown by the translation.
(17) ɔ́ zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ákʷʊ́kʷɔ́ ɲé m̩̄
3SG buy-rV book give 1SG
'He bought a book and gave it to me./ He bought a book for me.'
(18) ɔ́ zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ákʷʊ́kʷɔ́ mà ɲé m̩̄ jā
3SG buy-rV book and give 1SG 3SG
'He bought a book (for himself), and he gave it to me.'
In sum, Igbo SVCs are different from (c)overt coordination syntactically and semantically. Syntactically, SVCs allow argument sharing. The future tense marker, progressive marker, and negation morpheme are only attached to the first verb of an SVC. Semantically, the two verbs in an SVC are interpreted as subparts of a single event. The contrast between SVCs and coordination demonstrates that SVCs exist in Igbo.
With the tests above, we identified five types of SVCs classified by the semantic relation between two verbs. They are (i) purpose SVCs as in (8), where the second verb denotes the purpose of the first verb, (ii) instrumental SVCs as in (10), where the first verb introduces an instrument for the action expressed by the second verb, (iii) manner SVCs as in (13), where the first describes the manner while the second verb indicates the action, (iv) directional SVCs as in (16), where the second verb expresses a path of motion, and (v) benefactive SVCs as in (17). Among those types, purpose SVCs and benefactive SVCs have shared arguments and are expected by Collins (1997) to allow two verbs to form a V-V compound through incorporation. Also, in manner SVCs and directional SVCs, the first verb is always intransitive and can in principle form a verb compound with the second verb. The next section focuses on SVCs with possible compound counterparts to investigate the evidence for derivational relationship between V-V compounds and SVCs.
3. Relationship Between V-V Compounds and SVCs
The existence of both V-V compounds and SVCs in Igbo allows us to test the hypothesis of verb incorporation proposed by Collins (1997) through comparing their distributions. If compound verbs were derived by verb incorporation, we would observe systematic patterns between SVCs and V-V compounds. The comparison of SVCs and compound counterparts in four types of SVCs below, however, does not provide supporting evidence for a derivational relationship between them.
3.1. Purpose SVCs and Benefactive SVCs
First, both purpose SVCs and benefactive SVCs involve argument sharing, and the shared argument linearly occurs between two verbs. For purpose SVCs, we did not find any compound form based on the elicited data. For instance, the SVC in (19) cannot be expressed by a compound verb, as in (20).
(19) Àdá sì-ɹì ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀ ɹí-é
Ada cook-rV chicken eat-OVS
'Ada cooked a chicken and ate it.'
(20) *Àdá sì-ɹì-ɹì ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀
Ada cook-eat-rV chicken
Benefactive SVCs, conversely, have V-V compound forms. (21) and (22) illustrate that the two verbs “zʊ” (‘buy’) and “ɲe” (‘give’) can either form a compound or an SVC to express similar meanings.
(21) ɔ́ zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ákʷʊ́kʷɔ́ ɲē m̩̄
3SG buy-rV book give 1SG
'He bought a book and gave it to me.'
(22) ɔ́ zʊ̀-ɲè-ɹè m̩̀ ákʷʊ́kʷɔ́
3SG buy-give-rV 1SG book
'He bought a book for me.'
However, not all benefactive SVCs have compound counterparts. For instance, similar to “zʊ” (‘buy’), the meaning of “zu” (‘steal’) also involves acquirement. But according to our speaker, the SVC formed by “zu” with “ɲe” is marginal, shown in (23). Its corresponding compound verb is unacceptable, as in (24).
(23) ??ó zù-ɹù égō ɲé m̩̄
3SG steal-rV money give 1SG
'He stole money and gave it to me.'
(24) *ó zù-ɲè-ɹè m̩̀ égō
3SG steal-give-rV 1SG money
For verbs encoding production, such as “de” (‘write’) and “se” (‘draw’), their SVC forms are degraded but the compound forms are unacceptable, as in (25) and (26).
(25) ?ó dè-ɹè létà ɲé m̩̄
3SG write-rV letter give 1SG
'He wrote a letter and gave it to me.'
(26) *ó dè-ɲè-ɹè m̩̀ létà
3SG write-give-rV 1SG letter
On the other hand, some verbs can only form compounds with “ɲe” but not SVCs. A case in point is “g͡ba”. It means ‘get/fetch’ when used alone as in “ḿ̩ nà-à-g͡bà ḿ̩mīɹī” (‘I am getting water.’) When it forms a compound with “ɲe”, “g͡ba-ɲe” as a whole means ‘pour’, but this meaning cannot be expressed by an SVC, shown in (28).
(27) ḿ̩ g͡bà-ɲè-ɹè ìtè ḿ̩mīɹī
1SG get-give-rV pot water
'I poured water into a pot.'
(28) *ḿ̩ g͡bà ḿ̩mīɹī ɲé ìtè
1SG get water give pot
3.2. Manner SVCs and Directional SVCs
The other two types of SVCs that possibly have compound counterparts are manner SVCs and directional SVCs since they always have intransitives as the first verb. Previous studies have shown that a verb expressing motion can form a compound with a verb expressing direction , such as “bɪa-fe” (‘come across’).
According to our data, the relationship between directional SVCs and verb compounds is complicated. Some compounds consisting of directional verbs such as “g͡ba-ga” (‘run go’) and “g͡ba-pʊ” (‘run exit’) can be decomposed into SVCs, as in (29) and (30). In those SVCs, the first verb can have an optional cognate object after it.
(29) ɔ́ g͡bà-ɹà (ɔ́sɔ́) gá-á áhɪā
3SG run-rV race go-OVS market
'He ran to the market.'
(30) ɔ́ g͡bà-ɹà (ɔ́sɔ́) pʊ̀-ó nà-èzí ʊ́lɔ̀
3SG run-rV race exit-OVS outside house
'He ran outside a house.'
Other compounds require decomposed SVCs with a different word form. For instance, when “ga-ba” (‘walk enter’) is expressed by an SVC, the second verb needs to be changed to “baɲe”, as in (31).
(31) ɔ́ gà-ɹà (ídʒè) báɲé ná-ímé ʊ́lɔ̀
3SG walk-rV walk enter inside house
'He walked into a house.'
Further, there are directional compounds with hardly acceptable SVC counterparts, such as “ga-fe” (‘go across’) shown below.
(32) ɔ́ gā-fè-ɹè ʊ́lɔ̀
3SG go-cross-rV house
'He went past a house.'
(33) ??ɔ ga-ɹa (idʒe) fe-e ʊlɔ
3SG walk-rV walk cross-OVS house
Manner SVCs pattern with directional SVCs in having an optional cognate object after the first verb, but different from directional SVCs, they have no compound forms. For example, the two verbs in (34) cannot be incorporated into “kʷʊ-ge”.
(34) ɔ́ kʷʊ̀ (ɔ́tɔ́) é-gè égʷū
3SG stand stand ?-listen music
'He is standing listening to music.'
3.3. The Lack of Resultative SVCs
Aside from the SVCs discussed above, there is one type of SVCs occurring in other languages but absent from Igbo, namely, resultative SVCs. Amaechi (2013) considers the causative structure below as resultative SVCs . However, according to the definition of SVCs, each component of an SVC must be able to occur on its own right, but when “mere” is used on its own, such as “tʃiɔ́má mè-ɹè íhẽ́” (‘Chioma did something’), the causative meaning disappears.
(35) Adá mere dí ya arụọ ụlọ
Ada cause husband 3SG build house
'Ada made her husband build a house.'
Despite a lack of resultative SVCs, Igbo has resultative compounds, and those compound verbs can be decomposed into two sentences or coordination but not SVCs. We have two examples shown in (5) and (7) in section 2. (37) and (39) present two additional examples. In each case, there are two sentences expressing action and result respectively. The required subject in the second sentence shows that the SVC form is not allowed.
(36) Àdá tì-wà-ɹà éféɹé
Ada strike-break-rV plate
'Ada broke a plate.'
(37) Àdá tì-ɹì éféɹè nà àlà, *(ɔ́) wā-ā
Ada stike-rV plate on floor 3SG break-OVS
'Ada struck a plate on the floor and it broke.'
(38) ɔ́ gbā-bà-ɹà bɔ́ɔ̀lʊ̀ ná-ímé èkètè
3SG kick-enter-rV ball inside basket
'He kicked a ball into a basket.'
(39) ɔ́ gbà-ɹà bɔ́ɔ̀lʊ̀, *(ɔ́) bàɲé ná-ímé èkètè
3SG kick-rV ball 3SG enter inside basket
'He kicked a ball and it entered a basket.'
To sum up, in this section, we explored the derivational relationship between V-V compounds and SVCs by comparing their distribution for four types of SVCs. It turns out that manner SVCs and purpose SVCs do not have compound forms. Some directional SVCs and benefactive SVCs have compound counterparts, but their distribution is not systematically predictable. Also, there are compound verbs without an SVC counterpart, illustrated by the lack of resultative SVCs in Igbo. Those facts challenge the view that V-V compounds in Igbo are derived from SVCs through verb incorporation and further cast doubt on Collins' (1997) analysis.
The following section further demonstrates the incompatibility of Collins' (1997) analysis with SVCs in Igbo and proposes a double-headed structure based on the proposal by Baker (1989) and Hiraiwa & Bodomo (2008).
4. A Double-Headed Structure of SVCs in Igbo
4.1. The Incompatibility of a Control Structure
Collins (1997) analyzes SVCs as a control structure with a pro as in Figure 2 [10]. His arguments for the existence of a pro come from resultative SVCs in Ewe. For instance, the resultative in (40) allows an optional postposition “yi”, which is a oblique/default case assigner in Ewe. The existence of “yi” shows that there is an empty category assigned case in (40). Collins thus proposes a control structure for SVCs where a pro coindexed with the matrix object.
(40) Me nya ɖevi-ɛ dzo (yi)
1SG chase child-DEF leave P
'I chased the child away.'
However, the lack of resultative SVCs in Igbo poses a challenge to Collins' analysis. As is reported by Collins (1997), the case assigner “yi” is allowed in resultatives but not other SVCs with direct object sharing, such as (41). Therefore, there is a lack of evidence for the existence of empty category in Igbo SVCs
(41) Wo ɖa fufu ɖu (*yi)
3PL cook fufu eat
'They cooked fufu and ate it.'
Moreover, section 3 has established that the distribution of V-V compounds and SVCs would be unpredictable assuming the verb incorporation hypothesis, further invalidating Collin’s proposal. Furthermore, constituency tests provide additional arguments against a control structure for SVCs. As in (42), the object of the first verb cannot form a constituent with the second verbal phrase in a coordination structure, contrary to Collins' prediction with the structure in Figure 2.
(42) *tʃiɔ́má dʒì ḿ̩mà gbú-ó ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀ mà ósísí kʊ́-ɔ́ àdā
Chioma use knife kill-? chicken and stick hit-? Ada
Intended: 'Chioma used a knife to kill a chicken and a stick to hit Ada.'
On the other hand, we will see in section 4.2 that a double-headed structure can naturally account for the syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs.
4.2. Endocentricity and Double-Headedness in Igbo SVCs
Ebere & Agbo (2021) studies the semantic relationship of V-V compounds and claims that compound verbs in Igbo are semantically endocentric such that compounding verbal lexemes are of unequal semantic degree . Either the first or the second lexeme can be the semantic head, and the other is the modifier.
Since some V-V compounds can be decomposed into SVCs, the endocentric relation is also reflected between two verbs of an SVC. For instance, in the directional SVC in (29), repeated in (43), the first verb signals a manner of motion and serves as a semantic modifier of the second verbal phrase. One piece of evidence is that the meaning of (43) is mainly encoded by the second verb as the manner of motion can be an implicit argument shown in (44). Another piece of evidence is that the cognate object of the first verb can be optional to make it phonologically lighter, which is in contrast with an intransitive verb used alone, (43) vs. (45).
(43) ɔ́ g͡bà-ɹà (ɔ́sɔ́) gá-á áhɪā
3SG run-rV race go-OVS market
'He ran to the market.'
(44) ɔ́ gà-ɹà áhɪā
3SG go-rV market
'He went to the market.'
(45) ɔ́ g͡bà-ɹà *(ɔ́sɔ́)
3SG run-rV race
'He ran.'
Also, there are V-V compounds with head-modifier relation that can be decomposed into SVCs, such as benefactive SVCs in (21), repeated in (46). Here, the second verb is semantically dependent as it introduces a benefactive of the first verb. Moreover, the shared object can only occur after the first verb as shown in (47), indicating that the first verb is the semantic head of benefactive SVCs.
(16) ɔ́ zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ákʷʊ́kʷɔ́ ɲé m̩̄
3SG buy-rV book give 1SG
'He bought a book and gave it to me.'
(47) *ɔ́ zʊ̀-ɹʊ̀ ɲé ḿ̩ ákʷʊ́kʷɔ́
3SG buy-rV give 1SG book
'He bought a book and gave it to me.'
We observe that for other types of SVCs in Igbo, the two verbs are also not semantically equivalent. They either form a head-modifier or modifier-head relation. Manner SVCs, e.g., (48) and instrumental SVCs, e.g., (49) pattern with directional SVCs in that the first verb can be phonologically lighter (without inflection) and semantically dependent.
(48) ɔ́ kʷʊ̀ (ɔ́tɔ́) é-gè égʷū
3SG stand stand ?-listen music
'He is standing listening to music.'
(49) ɔ́ dʒì ḿ̩mà gbú-ó ɔ̄kʊ̄kɔ̀
3SG use knife kill-OVS chicken
'He used a knife to kill a chicken.'
Purpose SVCs, e.g., (50), on the other hand, pattern with benefactive SVCs in that the shared object only occurs after the first verb.
(50) Àdá sì-ɹì ɔ̀kʊ́kɔ̀ ɹí-é
Ada cook-rV chicken eat-OVS
'Ada cooked a chicken and ate it.'
We show that the endocentric property of Igbo SVCs comes naturally from a double-headed structure by Baker (1989). In his analysis, both verbs are syntactically independent of each other. Thus, either of them can be a potential semantic head. Therefore, we propose a double-headed symmetric structure for SVCs in Igbo. For modifier-head SVCs, as (48), we have a structure in Figure 4, where two verbs have their own arguments and both project to the higher V'.
Figure 4. The tree structure of (48).
For head-modifier SVCs with shared arguments, as in (50), the two verbs constitute a multi-dominance structure, as in Figure 5. The theme theta-role is assigned to the shared object by both verbs. And we assume, following Hiraiwa & Bodomo (2008), that the symmetric structure changed to an asymmetric structure through verb movement and object shift before Spell-Out for linearization.
Figure 5. The tree structure of (50).
5. Conclusion
In this paper, we studied the syntactic structure and semantic relationship between two verbs of SVCs in Igbo. First, we disentangled the confound between SVCs and covert coordination and demonstrated that both V-V compounds and SVCs exist in Igbo. In addition, The comparison between the distribution of V-V compounds and SVCs does not support a derivational relationship and challenged the hypothesis of verb incorporation. Further, the lack of resultatives invalidated the existence of a pro. In contrast, we demonstrated that the endocentric property of SVCs in Igbo come naturally from a double-headed structure. Thus, our novel data from Igbo SVCs provide support for double-headedness building on Baker (1989) and Hiraiwa & Bodomo (2008) but argue against a control structure by Collins (1997).
Abbreviations

SVC

Serial Verb Construction

V-V

Verb-Verb

Author Contributions
Jing Ji is the sole author. The author read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This work is not supported by any external funding.
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting the outcome of this research work has been reported in this manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
The author declares no conflicts of interest.
References
[1] Lovestrand, J. (2021). Serial verb constructions. Annual Review of Linguistics, 7(1), 109-130.
[2] Nyame, J., & Tomekyin, C. (2022). Serial Verb Constructions in Nzema: A Descriptive Analysis. Journal of Communication and Cultural Trends, 4(2), 81-101.
[3] Bodomo, A., & Abubakari, H. (2022). Serial verb reduplication in the Mabia languages of West Africa. Legon Journal of the Humanities, 33(2), 29-58.
[4] Kouankem, C. (2022). Issues on Serial Verb Constructions in Medumba. Language in Africa, 3(3), 40-66.
[5] ThankGod, L., & Ngulube, I. E. (2022). SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN ABUA. Linguistics, 5(1), 50-68.
[6] Tyler, M., & Kastner, I. (2022). Serial verb constructions and the syntax-prosody interface. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 1-22.
[7] Aikhenvald, A. Y. Serial verb constructions in typological perspective. Serial verb constructions: A cross-linguistic typology. 2006, 1-68.
[8] Baker, M. C. Object sharing and projection in serial verb constructions. Linguistic inquiry. 1989, 20(4), 513-553.
[9] Hiraiwa, K. and Bodomo, A. Object-sharing as symmetric sharing: Predicate clefting and serial verbs in dàgáárè. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. 2008, 26(4), 795-832.
[10] Collins, C. Argument sharing in serial verb constructions. Linguistic inquiry. 1997, 461-497.
[11] Amaechi, M. Case checking in igbo serial verb constructions. Research on Humanities and Social Sciences. 2013, 3(9).
[12] Obiamalu, G. O. and Mbagwu, D. Semantax of v+ nye verbal complex in igbo. Journal of West African Languages. 2014, 41(2), 34.
[13] Lord, C. How igbo got from sov serializing to svo compounding. Studies in African Linguistics, Supplement. 1977, 7, 145-55.
[14] Bisang, W. Serial verb constructions. Language and Linguistics Compass. 2009, 3(3), 792-814.
[15] Ebere, D. F. and Agbo, M. S. Endocentricity in igbo vv compounds. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGES, LINGUISTICS AND LITERARY STUDIES (JOLLS). 2021, 10(2), 73.
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Ji, J. (2024). Serial Verbs Constructions as Double-headed Syntactic Structures: Evidence from Igbo. International Journal of Language and Linguistics, 12(5), 187-193. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Ji, J. Serial Verbs Constructions as Double-headed Syntactic Structures: Evidence from Igbo. Int. J. Lang. Linguist. 2024, 12(5), 187-193. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Ji J. Serial Verbs Constructions as Double-headed Syntactic Structures: Evidence from Igbo. Int J Lang Linguist. 2024;12(5):187-193. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13,
      author = {Jing Ji},
      title = {Serial Verbs Constructions as Double-headed Syntactic Structures: Evidence from Igbo
    },
      journal = {International Journal of Language and Linguistics},
      volume = {12},
      number = {5},
      pages = {187-193},
      doi = {10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ijll.20241205.13},
      abstract = {Although serial verb constructions (SVCs) are widely observed across languages, there is ongoing debate regarding their syntactic representation, particularly in cases involving shared arguments. Two prominent approaches to SVCs with argument sharing are the double-headed structure proposed by Baker (1989) and the control structure suggested by Collins (1997). This paper presents novel data from Igbo to evaluate these differing approaches. First, we disentangle the confound between covert coordination and SVCs through syntactic and semantic tests, demonstrating that SVCs do exist in Igbo. We then conduct a comprehensive comparison between SVCs and corresponding verb-verb (V-V) compounds. The absence of a systematic derivational relationship between SVCs and V-V compounds raises doubts about the verb incorporation mechanism posited by Collins. Furthermore, the lack of resultative SVCs in Igbo challenges the existence of an empty category, further undermining the control structure for SVCs. In contrast, we illustrate that the endocentric property of Igbo SVCs aligns naturally with Baker's double-headed syntactic structure. Building on this, we propose a double-headed structure for SVCs in Igbo, both with and without shared arguments. Our findings thus provide a novel argument for a permissible structure of SVCs with argument sharing and for the availability of double-headedness in Universal Grammar.
    },
     year = {2024}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Serial Verbs Constructions as Double-headed Syntactic Structures: Evidence from Igbo
    
    AU  - Jing Ji
    Y1  - 2024/11/22
    PY  - 2024
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13
    DO  - 10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13
    T2  - International Journal of Language and Linguistics
    JF  - International Journal of Language and Linguistics
    JO  - International Journal of Language and Linguistics
    SP  - 187
    EP  - 193
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2330-0221
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20241205.13
    AB  - Although serial verb constructions (SVCs) are widely observed across languages, there is ongoing debate regarding their syntactic representation, particularly in cases involving shared arguments. Two prominent approaches to SVCs with argument sharing are the double-headed structure proposed by Baker (1989) and the control structure suggested by Collins (1997). This paper presents novel data from Igbo to evaluate these differing approaches. First, we disentangle the confound between covert coordination and SVCs through syntactic and semantic tests, demonstrating that SVCs do exist in Igbo. We then conduct a comprehensive comparison between SVCs and corresponding verb-verb (V-V) compounds. The absence of a systematic derivational relationship between SVCs and V-V compounds raises doubts about the verb incorporation mechanism posited by Collins. Furthermore, the lack of resultative SVCs in Igbo challenges the existence of an empty category, further undermining the control structure for SVCs. In contrast, we illustrate that the endocentric property of Igbo SVCs aligns naturally with Baker's double-headed syntactic structure. Building on this, we propose a double-headed structure for SVCs in Igbo, both with and without shared arguments. Our findings thus provide a novel argument for a permissible structure of SVCs with argument sharing and for the availability of double-headedness in Universal Grammar.
    
    VL  - 12
    IS  - 5
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Department of Chinese Language and Literature, Guangdong Baiyun University, Guangzhou, China

    Biography: Jing Ji is a lecturer in the Department of Chinese Language and Literature at Guangdong Baiyun University. She graduated from the Department of Linguistics of McGill University in 2024. She also obtained a Master in Computational Linguistics from Stony Brook University in 2020. She has been working on multiple areas of linguistics and presented her work at top conferences, such as NELS and Amsterdam Colloquium.

    Research Fields: Syntax, semantics, computational linguistics, prosody, field work